The possibility that language shapes memory and knowledge
has intrigued scholars across various disciplines, including psychology,
sociology, philosophy, linguistics, anthropology, and cultural studies.
Everyday experiences illustrate how describing an event can lead to distinct
mental representations and memories of it. For instance, verbalizing an
incident using different expressions can result in different memory
representations. Similarly, how an event is labeled in the mass media can evoke
varied representations and evaluations, influencing how it's remembered later.
In historical contexts, memories of the same events can
differ significantly depending on the language used to describe them. For
example, nineteenth-century North American events may be remembered differently
when labeled as either "the pioneering frontier movement" or
"the expulsion and murder of the First Nations." Additionally,
linguistic categories influence memory retention; languages lacking specific
categories for certain stimuli may result in poorer memory recall compared to
languages with more descriptive tools.
Language, as a culturally embedded system, not only
reflects but also shapes cultural memory. This article explores theoretical
approaches, key concepts, and empirical evidence related to the influence of
language on memory at the individual and interpersonal levels. While primarily
focusing on cognitive processes, it also considers pragmatic functions and
social processes inherent in dialogical communication.
Foundational Issues: The Relationship between Language
and Memory: Memory encompasses various processes and structures that allow past
experiences to influence present experiences and behaviors. Language, on the
other hand, is considered a symbolic system for representing experiences in a
communicable format. The relationship between language and memory can be
examined in two ways: how language shapes memory and how memory influences
language. This chapter concentrates on the former approach, treating memory as
the dependent variable and language as one of the independent variables.
Different perspectives exist regarding the relationship
between language and cognition. Some argue that language is integral to human
thinking and memory, while others believe that cognition transcends language.
The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis suggests that language determines cognition, while
opposing views assert the independence of cognitive processes from linguistic
form. These contrasting viewpoints highlight the complex interplay between
language and memory.
While the positions on the relationship between language
and memory may seem contradictory, psychological research suggests a middle
ground that provides productive insights into how language shapes memory. On
one hand, some cognitive processes seem to operate independently of language.
For example, pre-linguistic infants exhibit higher cognitive capacities, and
individuals with severe language disorders often retain intact cognitive
functions. Additionally, non-verbal modes of cognition, such as visual,
spatial, auditory, olfactory, or motor information, play a crucial role in
mental representation. Certain memories, termed non-declarative, occur outside
conscious awareness and are difficult to verbalize, such as motor skills or
implicit influences of previous exposure to stimuli.
On the other hand, language is a fundamental tool for
interpreting and retaining experiences. Some mental representations are
impossible without language, particularly abstract or higher-level concepts.
Declarative memory, which includes semantic and episodic memory, is amenable to
verbalization, even though episodic memory is often constructed from
non-linguistic representations. Thus, while cognition and memory may transcend
language in some instances, language remains central to many mental processes.
Considering these factors, the debate about whether
thought depends on language or vice versa appears futile. Instead,
understanding how non-linguistic and linguistic representations interact in the
mind is more psychologically relevant. Different research areas on language
effects on memory emphasize the role of different levels of representation. For
instance, verbal overshadowing examines how memory is influenced when
attempting to describe stimuli primarily processed non-linguistically, such as
faces or tastes.
2
The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis has been influential in
stimulating research on the effects of language on cognition. While the strong
version of the hypothesis, proposing a causal relationship between language and
cognition, has been largely abandoned, weaker versions suggesting a
non-deterministic relation are still debated. Studies on color memory, for
example, investigate whether differences in color terms across languages affect
memory performance for color stimuli. Despite initial skepticism, research has
shown that language can indeed influence cognition, particularly for
experiences that are abstract or culturally mediated. Therefore, understanding
the interplay between language and memory involves considering how linguistic
structures shape cognitive processes across different domains.
In a related area of research, scholars have explored how
bilingualism, the ability to use two languages regularly in daily life,
influences memory. In a study by Marian and Neisser, Russian-English bilinguals
recalled more experiences from their Russian-speaking period when interviewed
in Russian, and vice versa when interviewed in English. This suggests that the
linguistic environment during retrieval influences memory accessibility,
aligning with the concept of context-dependent memory. This phenomenon
indicates that belonging to different language communities can shape how
individuals remember their personal past, with potential implications for
cultural memory.
Moving on to within-language approaches, linguistic
differences within the same language can also impact memory. Various studies
have examined how verbalization affects memory encoding and retrieval.
Verbalization can either enhance or hinder memory, depending on factors such as
the ease of verbal description and the nature of the stimuli. For example,
verbalizing complex visual stimuli may interfere with memory accuracy, while
providing verbal labels for stimuli can shape mental representation and
subsequent memory. Additionally, differences in the way people encode or
retrieve information verbally can lead to biases in memory recall.
Pragmatic aspects of communication, such as audience tuning
and socially shared reality, also influence memory. Audience tuning refers to
the process of adjusting language use based on the audience's perspective,
knowledge, and attitudes. Studies employing the saying-is-believing paradigm
have demonstrated that aligning language with the audience's attitudes can not
only bias speakers' messages but also affect their subsequent memory and
cognitive representations of the message topic. This phenomenon suggests that
communicators may end up believing and remembering what they said to align with
their audience's attitudes, rather than the original information they learned.
Crucially, the impact of audience-tuning on memory
appears to be heavily influenced by the motives and context of interpersonal
communication. Echterhoff and colleagues suggest that such effects arise when
communicators jointly construct a socially shared reality with their audience
regarding the topic or target. Shared reality imbues previously uncertain
representations of experience with subjective validity and is indicated by
individuals' trust in the co-constructor(s). In this view, communicators reduce
uncertainty about the target person by considering the audience's attitude when
crafting a message about them. Supporting this shared-reality perspective,
research by Echterhoff, Higgins, Kopietz, and Groll demonstrated memory biases
when audience tuning aimed at creating shared reality, but not when motivated
by non-epistemic goals such as obtaining a monetary incentive. Moreover,
communicators tuning to an in-group audience, whom they trusted as appropriate
co-constructors, incorporated the audience-tuned view into their memory, while
those addressing an out-group audience, whom they distrusted, did not. Notably,
overt verbal messages and message content at recall did not vary between
shared-reality conditions. Instead, what differed was the communicators'
perception of the validity of their message in conveying a genuine view about
the target. Thus, mere differences in verbal descriptions or linguistic
representations may not always suffice for effects on subsequent memory.
Instead, communicators' motives and relationship with the audience play a
critical role in shaping their mental representations of the topic.
This research also carries implications for the formation
of cultural knowledge. Verbal communication can subtly disseminate knowledge,
memories, and beliefs when individuals actively engage as communicators, not
just passive recipients, provided they adhere to common rules such as audience
tuning and jointly create a shared reality with their communication partners.
Within homogeneous groups or communities where members consider each other
trustworthy co-constructors of reality in the face of uncertainty, such
communication may reinforce and even amplify existing worldviews, including
prevalent cultural stereotypes. Consequently, it may contribute to conflict
with out-groups. Future research should aim to further integrate these
cognitive, motivational, and socio-cultural dimensions to elucidate how verbal
communication shapes memory and knowledge.
No comments:
Post a Comment