Memory serves as the foundation for both personal and
collective identity, operating on three distinct levels. At its innermost
level, memory functions within our neuro-mental system, constituting our
personal memory. On the social level, memory becomes a matter of communication
and interaction, as demonstrated by Maurice Halbwachs, who emphasized the
social nature of memory. Meanwhile, psychoanalysts like Sigmund Freud and Carl
Gustav Jung explored collective memory within the human psyche's unconscious depths.
Aby Warburg expanded the concept of memory to the
cultural realm, introducing the term "social memory." He examined how
cultural artifacts, particularly images, serve as carriers of memory. Warburg's
"Mnemosyne" project explored the enduring influence of classical
antiquity on Western culture. Similarly, Thomas Mann's novels delved into
cultural memory, focusing on the Late Bronze Age in Palestine and Egypt while
reflecting on European cultural heritage and its Jewish roots.
The distinction between "communicative memory"
and "cultural memory" clarifies the relationship between memory and
tradition. While Halbwachs focused on collective memory as shaped by social
interactions, cultural memory extends beyond communication to encompass
institutionalized forms of memory preservation and transmission. External
symbols, such as monuments and museums, play a crucial role in shaping cultural
memory, serving as reminders across generations.
Unlike cultural memory, communicative memory lacks
institutional support and relies on everyday interactions and communication. It
is sustained by familial and communal ties and tends to have a limited time
depth, typically spanning three generations. The durability of communicative
memory depends on the continuity of social bonds and frameworks of
communication.
Halbwachs's later work highlighted the institutional and
power dynamics involved in shaping collective memory. His examination of
Christian memory in Palestine during the Roman Empire's adoption of
Christianity revealed the influence of theological dogma and the church's power
structure on the construction of official memory.
Jan Vansina, an anthropologist renowned for his work with
oral societies in Africa, identified a tripartite structure in the
representation of the past within these societies. He observed that recent
events, prominently featured in everyday communication, gradually recede into
the background over time. Information about the recent past becomes scarcer and
less precise as time passes. In contrast, the most distant past, including
narratives about the world's origins and the tribe's early history, is
extensively formalized and institutionalized, preserved through rituals, songs,
and other cultural practices. This formalized aspect of memory, termed
"cultural memory," involves specialized individuals and requires
specific occasions for its activation.
Vansina's concept of the "floating gap"
highlights the divide between the informal memory of recent events and the
formalized cultural memory of the distant past. This gap shifts with each
generation, influencing historical consciousness in oral societies, which
typically operates on two levels: the time of origins and the recent past.
In oral history, the communicative memory pertains to
memories of the recent past shared among contemporaries. This aligns with
Halbwachs's notion of collective memory, focusing on memories passed down
through oral tradition rather than written sources. Cultural memory, on the
other hand, relies on fixed points in the past, represented through symbols and
rituals that illuminate the present. In cultural memory, the distinction
between myth and history blurs, emphasizing the subjective interpretation of
the past rather than its objective reconstruction.
Memory, particularly cultural memory, is closely tied to
identity, shaping individuals' sense of self within various social, cultural,
and political contexts. Memory serves as a repository of identity, reflecting
one's diachronic identity as an individual or member of a group. The dynamics
of memory formation are influenced by affective ties and social obligations,
driving individuals to remember in order to maintain a sense of belonging.
Conversely, the transition from one group to another often involves forgetting
the memories associated with the original identity, highlighting the complex
interplay between memory and identity formation.
The distinction between communicative and cultural memory
is also evident in the social dynamics of participation. In communicative memory,
group participation is diffuse, with individuals acquiring knowledge through
everyday interaction and socialization. There are no specialized custodians of
informal memory; rather, knowledge is collectively shared among community
members. Conversely, participation in cultural memory is highly differentiated,
even within oral and egalitarian societies. Historically, the preservation of
cultural memory was entrusted to specialized individuals such as poets or
griots, who served as guardians of the group's cultural heritage.
These cultural memory specialists, found in both oral and
literate societies, include a diverse array of figures such as shamans, bards,
priests, and scholars. In oral traditions, the specialization of memory
carriers depends on the demands placed on their memory, with some rituals
requiring verbatim transmission and strict adherence to oral scripts. For
instance, in ancient Rwanda, the memorization of ritual texts was a highly
esteemed task, with specialists risking severe consequences for errors.
Rituals often gave rise to mnemonic techniques, such as
knotting cords or other forms of pre-writing, to aid in memorization.
Interestingly, attitudes toward writing varied among different religious
traditions. While some, like the Indo-European traditions, distrusted writing
due to fears of dissemination and loss of secrecy, others, such as ancient Near
Eastern societies like Mesopotamia, embraced writing as a means of codifying
and transmitting sacred knowledge.
Moreover, cultural memory participation is often
structured by notions of secrecy and esotericism, leading to elitism within
society. Certain areas of knowledge are restricted to select individuals,
requiring initiation or specialized training for access. This inherent tendency
toward elitism results in systematic exclusions, such as the exclusion of women
in certain historical contexts or lower classes in societies dominated by an
educated bourgeoisie.
Additionally, the media of cultural memory often exhibit
a form of intra-cultural diglossia, reflecting the distinction between a
"great tradition" and several "little traditions."
Traditional societies commonly use different languages or linguistic varieties
for formal versus everyday communication. However, modern societies introduce
further linguistic varieties through cultural media like film and television,
complicating this binary structure.
The dynamics of cultural memory are marked by transitions
and transformations, which hold structural significance and merit brief discussion.
One direction involves the transition from autobiographical and communicative
memory to cultural memory. Another direction within cultural memory involves
moving from the background to the foreground, from the periphery to the center,
and from latency or potentiality to manifestation or actualization, and vice
versa. These shifts entail crossing structural boundaries: the boundary between
embodied and mediated forms of memory, and the boundary between what we term
"working" and "reference memories," or "canon"
and "archive."
No comments:
Post a Comment